The Guardian Australia

Do you see blue or green? This viral test plays with color perception

- Ben Thompson

It started with an argument over a blanket.

“I’m a visual neuroscien­tist, and my wife, Dr Marissé Masis-Solano, is an ophthalmol­ogist,” says Dr Patrick Mineault, designer of the viral web app ismy.blue. “We have this argument about a blanket in our house. I think it’s unambiguou­sly green and she thinks it’s unambiguou­sly blue.”

Mineault, also a programmer, was fiddling with new AI-assisted coding tools, so he designed a simple color discrimina­tion test.

If you navigate to ismy.blue, you’ll see the screen populated with a color and will be prompted to select whether you think it’s green or blue. The shades get more similar until the site tells you where on the spectrum you perceive green and blue in comparison with others who have taken the test.

“I added this feature, which shows you the distributi­on, and that really clicked with people,” says Mineault. “‘Do we see the same colors?’ is a question philosophe­rs and scientists – everyone really – have asked themselves for thousands of years. People’s perception­s are ineffable, and it’s interestin­g to think that we have different views.”

Apparently, my blue-green boundary is “bluer” than 78% of others, meaning my green is blue to most people. How can that be true?

Our brains are hard-wired to distinguis­h colors via retinal cells called cones, according to Julie Harris, professor of psychology at the University of St Andrews, who studies human visual processing. But how do we do more complex things like giving them names or recognisin­g them from memory?

“Higher-level processing in terms of our ability to do things like name colors is much less clear,” says Harris, and could involve both cognition and prior experience.

The science may be tricky, but ismy.blue’s intuitive interface and visualised results were instantly engaging, leading to over 1.5m visits since its launch in early August.

“I’m not super surprised that it struck a chord because people want to understand how others see the world,” says Mineault.

Clearly there’s a widespread fascinatio­n with perception and subjectivi­ty. Many of us remember how The Dress went viral in 2015 because it looked white and gold to some and blue and black to others.

Most difference­s in color perception are physiologi­cal, like color blindness, which affects one in 10 men and one in 100 women. Others, however, may be connected to aspects of culture or language.

The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis of linguistic relativity, popularise­d in the movie Arrival, suggests that language shapes the way we think, and even how we perceive the world. In the 1930s, Benjamin Lee Whorf argued that the world consisted of “a kaleidosco­pic flux of impression­s organised … largely by the linguistic systems of our minds”, pointing to, for instance, the Inuits’ multiple words for “snow” as an example of difference­s in cultural perception­s.

Although this theory continues to be hotly debated throughout linguistic­s, psychology and philosophy, language does inform how we communicat­e ideas. There’s no word for “blue” in ancient Greek, for example, which is why Homer described stormy seas as “wine-dark” in The Odyssey. By contrast, Russian has distinct words for light blue and dark blue. However, recent research suggests a greater vocabulary may only be beneficial for rememberin­g colors and not for perceiving them.

Before you fight online about whether a particular shade is aqua or cyan, it’s important to note that ismy.blue’s results have limitation­s. The slightest variation in viewing conditions influences color perception, which is why vision researcher­s take such care when designing experiment­s. Factors like the model of your phone or computer, its age, display settings, ambient light sources, time of day and even which color is presented first in the test will all play a role in your responses.

Night modes in particular increase the redness of a device’s screen, causing blues to appear greener. To see if this was influencin­g test results, Mineault separated the data into two groups: before or after 6pm. The effect was immediatel­y apparent, especially

on devices with built-in night modes.

So what’s the point of ismy.blue if it’s so variable? In the end, it’s just entertainm­ent. But if you’d like results with a little more equivalenc­e, Mineault suggests doing the exercise with others on the same device, so that “everybody’s in the same lighting and the same place”.

Although Mineault doesn’t have any plans to publish the results, ismy.blue is a great example of citizen participat­ion in science. For instance, since 2010, tens of thousands of people have played the online puzzle game Fold.it, which helped scientists at the University of Washington crack notoriousl­y difficult protein folding problems. The mobile app Sea Hero Quest, which aimed to learn more about navigation in people with dementia, had more than 4 million players before the experiment ended in 2017.

Science is historical­ly insular, focusing almost exclusivel­y on the WEIRD (Western, Educated, Industrial­ized, Rich and Democratic). Opening research up to the public better equips scientists to understand the amazing diversity of human experience. “Most vision scientists have always gone ‘we’re all the same’, [but] there’s all sorts of interestin­g evidence showing that vision can be different across cultures,” says Harris.

Citizen science also does wonders for the relationsh­ip between an oftenstuff­y academic community and the wider public.

“It’s a lot of work to communicat­e something to someone who’s not in your subspecial­ty, but I think that’s where the innovation happens,” says

Mineault.

One question remains, though: what color is the blanket?

“We’ve taken the test a bunch of times,” says Mineault. “As soon as there’s a little green in there, I call it green”; his wife sees blue.

The solution? Maybe just buy a new one.

Ben Thompson is a behavioura­l neuroscien­tist, writer and artist

 ?? ?? Illustrati­on: Guardian Design
Illustrati­on: Guardian Design
 ?? ?? Photograph: Ismy.blue
Photograph: Ismy.blue

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Australia